Dogs may really understand the names of their toys

Unsurprisingly, your dog can learn to sit when you say “sit” and come when called. However, a study has discovered that dogs generally also know that certain words “stand for” certain objects. When dogs hear those words, brain activity recordings suggest activating a matching mental representation.

“Dogs do not only react with a learned behaviour to certain words,” says Marianna Boros (@FamDogProject) of the Department of Ethology at the Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary, one of the paper’s co-first authors. “They also don’t just associate that word with an object based on temporal contiguity without really understanding the meaning of those words, but they activate a memory of an object when they hear its name.”

The researchers say that word understanding tests with individuals who do not speak, such as infants and animals, usually require active choice. They’re asked to show or get an object after hearing its name. Very few dogs do well on such tests in the lab, often fetching objects correctly at a rate expected by chance.

The researchers wanted to look closer at dogs’ implicit understanding of object words by measuring brain activity using non-invasive EEG without asking them to act. The idea was that this might offer a more sensitive measure of their understanding of language.

In their studies, 18 dog owners said words for toys their dogs knew and then presented the objects to them. Sometimes, they presented a matching toy, while other times, they presented an object that didn’t match. For example, an owner would say, “Zara, look, the ball,” and present the object while the dog’s brain activity was captured on EEG.

The brain recording results showed a different pattern in the brain when the dogs were shown a matching object versus a mismatched one. That’s similar to what researchers have seen in humans and is widely accepted as evidence that they understand the words. The researchers also found a greater difference in those patterns for words that dogs knew better, offering further support for their understanding of object words. Interestingly, while the researchers thought this ability might depend on having a large vocabulary of object words, their findings showed that it doesn’t.

“Because typical dogs learn instruction words rather than object names, and there are only a handful of dogs with a large vocabulary of object words, we expected that dogs’ capacity for referential understanding of object words will be linked to the number of object words they know; but it wasn’t,” says Lilla Magyari, also of Eötvös Loránd University and University of Stavanger and the other co-first author.

“It doesn’t matter how many object words a dog understands—known words activate mental representations anyway, suggesting that this ability is generally present in dogs and not just in some exceptional individuals who know the names of many objects,” Boros added.

The researchers say the discovery that dogs as a species may generally have a capacity to understand words in a referential way, just as humans do, might reshape the way scientists think about the uniqueness of how humans use and understand language. That has important implications for theories and models of language evolution. It’s also an important realisation for dog owners.

“Your dog understands more than he or she shows signs of,” Magyari says. “Dogs are not merely learning a specific behaviour to certain words, but they might actually understand the meaning of some individual words as humans do.”

Image: Grzegorz Eliasiewicz

Search this website